Your browser doesn't support javascript.
loading
Mostrar: 20 | 50 | 100
Resultados 1 - 20 de 37
Filtrar
1.
Hum Psychopharmacol ; 39(3): e2896, 2024 May.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-38353526

RESUMO

OBJECTIVE: Stimuli received beyond a very short timeframe, known as temporal binding windows (TBWs), are perceived as separate events. In previous audio-visual multisensory integration (McGurk effect) studies, widening of TBWs has been observed in people with schizophrenia. The present study aimed to determine if dexamphetamine could increase TBWs in unimodal auditory and unimodal visual illusions that may have some validity as experimental models for auditory and visual hallucinations in psychotic disorders. METHODS: A double-blind, placebo-controlled, counter-balanced crossover design with permuted block randomisation for drug order was followed. Dexamphetamine (0.45 mg/kg, PO, q.d.) was administered to healthy participants. Phantom word illusion (speech illusion) and visual-induced flash illusion/VIFI (visual illusion) tests were measured to determine if TBWs were altered as a function of delay between stimuli presentations. Word emotional content for phantom word illusions was also analysed. RESULTS: Dexamphetamine significantly increased the total number of phantom words/speech illusions (p < 0.01) for pooled 220-1100 ms ISIs in kernel density estimation and the number of positive valence words heard (beta = 2.20, 95% CI [1.86, 2.55], t = 12.46, p < 0.001) with a large effect size (std. beta = 1.05, 95% CI [0.89, 1.22]) relative to placebo without affecting the TBWs. For the VIFI test, kernel density estimation for pooled 0-801 ms ISIs showed a significant difference (p < 0.01) in the data distributions of number of target flash (es) perceived by participants after receiving dexamphetamine as compared with placebo. CONCLUSIONS: Overall, healthy participants who were administered dexamphetamine (0.45 mg/kg, PO, q.d.) experienced increases in auditory and visual illusions in both phantom word illusion and VIFI tests without affecting their TBWs.


Assuntos
Estudos Cross-Over , Dextroanfetamina , Ilusões , Percepção Visual , Humanos , Método Duplo-Cego , Masculino , Adulto , Feminino , Ilusões/efeitos dos fármacos , Ilusões/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem , Dextroanfetamina/farmacologia , Dextroanfetamina/administração & dosagem , Percepção Visual/efeitos dos fármacos , Percepção Visual/fisiologia , Alucinações/induzido quimicamente , Fatores de Tempo , Estimulação Luminosa/métodos , Estimulantes do Sistema Nervoso Central/farmacologia , Estimulantes do Sistema Nervoso Central/administração & dosagem , Estimulação Acústica , Percepção da Fala/efeitos dos fármacos , Percepção Auditiva/efeitos dos fármacos , Percepção Auditiva/fisiologia , Adolescente
2.
PLoS One ; 18(7): e0287538, 2023.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-37440493

RESUMO

Studies that examined the effect of amphetamine or caffeine on spatial working memory (SWM) and verbal working memory (VWM) have used various tasks. However, there are no studies that have used spatial span tasks (SSTs) to assess the SWM effect of amphetamine and caffeine, although some studies have used digit span tasks (DST) to assess VWM. Previous reports also showed that increasing dopamine increases psychosis-like experiences (PLE, or schizotypy) scores which are in turn negatively associated with WM performance in people with high schizotypy and people with schizophrenia. Therefore, the present study aimed to examine the influence of d-amphetamine (0.45 mg/kg, PO), a dopamine releasing stimulant, on SST, DST, and on PLE in healthy volunteers. In a separate study, we examined the effect of caffeine, a nonspecific adenosine receptor antagonist with stimulant properties, on similar tasks. METHODS: Healthy participants (N = 40) took part in two randomized, double-blind, counter-balanced placebo-controlled cross-over pilot studies: The first group (N = 20) with d-amphetamine (0.45 mg/kg, PO) and the second group (N = 20) with caffeine (200 mg, PO). Spatial span and digit span were examined under four delay conditions (0, 2, 4, 8 s). PLE were assessed using several scales measuring various aspects of psychosis and schizotypy. RESULTS: We failed to find an effect of d-amphetamine or caffeine on SWM or VWM, relative to placebo. However, d-amphetamine increased a composite score of psychosis-like experiences (p = 0.0005), specifically: Scores on Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale, Perceptual Aberrations Scale, and Magical Ideation Scale were increased following d-amphetamine. The degree of change in PLE following d-amphetamine negatively and significantly correlated with changes in SWM, mainly at the longest delay condition of 8 s (r = -0.58, p = 0.006). CONCLUSION: The present results showed that moderate-high dose of d-amphetamine and moderate dose of caffeine do not directly affect performances on DST or SST. However, the results indicate that d-amphetamine indirectly influences SWM, through its effect on psychosis-like experiences. CLINICAL TRIAL REGISTRATION NUMBER: CT-2018-CTN-02561 (Therapeutic Goods Administration Clinical Trial Registry) and ACTRN12618001292268 (The Australian New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry) for caffeine study, and ACTRN12608000610336 for d-amphetamine study.


Assuntos
Cafeína , Dextroanfetamina , Humanos , Dextroanfetamina/farmacologia , Cafeína/farmacologia , Voluntários Saudáveis , Dopamina , Austrália , Anfetamina/farmacologia , Método Duplo-Cego
3.
J Psychiatry Neurosci ; 48(2): E90-E98, 2023.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36918195

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: The pathophysiology of psychosis is complex, but a better understanding of stimulus binding windows (BWs) could help to improve our knowledge base. Previous studies have shown that dopamine release is associated with psychosis and widened BWs. We can probe BW mechanisms using drugs of specific interest to psychosis. Therefore, we were interested in understanding how manipulation of the dopamine or catecholamine systems affect psychosis and BWs. We aimed to investigate the effect of dexamphetamine, as a dopamine-releasing stimulant, on the BWs in a unimodal illusion: the tactile funneling illusion (TFI). METHODS: We conducted a randomized, double-blind, counterbalanced placebo-controlled crossover study to investigate funnelling and errors of localization. We administered dexamphetamine (0.45 mg/kg) to 46 participants. We manipulated 5 spatial (5-1 cm) and 3 temporal (0, 500 and 750 ms) conditions in the TFI. RESULTS: We found that dexamphetamine increased funnelling illusion (p = 0.009) and increased the error of localization in a delay-dependent manner (p = 0.03). We also found that dexamphetamine significantly increased the error of localization at 500 ms temporal separation and 4 cm spatial separation (p interaction = 0.009; p 500ms|4cm v. baseline = 0.01). LIMITATIONS: Although amphetamine-induced models of psychosis are a useful approach to understanding the physiology of psychosis related to dopamine hyperactivity, dexamphetamine is equally effective at releasing noradrenaline and dopamine, and, therefore, we were unable to tease apart the effects of the 2 systems on BWs in our study. CONCLUSION: We found that dexamphetamine increases illusory perception on the unimodal TFI in healthy participants, which suggests that dopamine or other catecholamines have a role in increasing tactile spatial and temporal BWs.


Assuntos
Dextroanfetamina , Ilusões , Humanos , Dextroanfetamina/farmacologia , Dopamina/metabolismo , Estudos Cross-Over , Voluntários Saudáveis , Catecolaminas
4.
Hum Psychopharmacol ; 38(2): e2862, 2023 03.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36799101

RESUMO

OBJECTIVES: Our team previously showed that like the experience of the rubber hand illusion (RHI) in people with schizophrenia and their offspring¸ dexamphetamine administration to healthy volunteers increases the stimulus binding windows (BWs) in RHI. It is not clear if similar expansions of BWs are present for unimodal illusions. Studies have also shown that subjective or objective effects of amphetamine would be linked to between-person variations in personality measures. Therefore, we aimed to examine the effect of dexamphetamine (DEX), a dopamine-releasing stimulant, on illusory perception using unimodal sensory stimuli (Tactile Funneling Illusion [TFI]) across both temporal and spatial variables. We further examined the relationship between changes in psychometric scores and changes in illusion perception induced by dexamphetamine. METHODS: Healthy subjects (N = 20) participated in a randomized, double-blind, counter-balanced, placebo-controlled, cross-over study. The effects of dexamphetamine (0.45 mg/kg, PO, q.d.) on funneling and error of spatial localization (EL) were examined using TFI. Psychotomimetic effects were assessed using a battery of psychological measures. RESULTS: Dexamphetamine did not significantly increased the funneling illusion (p = 0.88) or EL (p = 0.5), relative to placebo. However, the degree of change in psychometric scores following dexamphetamine positively correlated with changes in funneling (ρ = 0.48, p = 0.03, n = 20), mainly at 0 ms delay condition (ρ = 0.6, p = 0.004, n = 20). CONCLUSION: Unlike multimodal illusions, alteration of BWs does not occur for unimodal illusions after administration of a dopamine-releasing agent. However, our findings indicate that moderate release of dopamine, through its psychotomimetic effect, indirectly influences unimodal illusion.


Assuntos
Ilusões , Percepção do Tato , Humanos , Estudos Cross-Over , Dopamina/farmacologia , Psicometria , Dextroanfetamina/farmacologia , Percepção Visual
5.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-36201240

RESUMO

Background: Memory impairments and psychosis-like experiences can be adverse effects of cannabis use. However, reports on the cognitive impact of cannabis use are not consistent. There are also limited studies on the psychotomimetic effects of cannabinoid compounds to reveal the association between cannabis and psychosis. Therefore, we investigated the effect of acute cannabinoid intoxication on verbal working memory (VWM) and spatial working memory (SWM) following oral doses of the synthetic cannabinoid agonist, nabilone (1-2 mg, oral). We further investigated the effect of nabilone on psychosis-like experiences (schizotypy scores) and associations of schizotypy with VWM and SWM. Methods: Healthy participants (n=28) completed spatial and digit span tasks across different delay conditions (0, 6, 12, and 18 sec) after receiving nabilone (1-2 mg, PO) or placebo in a randomized, double-blind, counterbalanced, crossover manner. A subset of participants completed a short battery of schizotypy measures (n=25). Results: Nabilone impaired VWM (p=0.03, weak effect size η2=0.02) and SWM (p=0.00016, η2=0.08). Nabilone did not significantly change overall schizotypy scores. Schizotypy scores were negatively correlated with working memory (WM) averaged across all delays and both modalities, under placebo (ρ=-0.41, p=0.04). In addition, there were significant negative correlations between occasions of cannabis use and overall WM averaged scores across drug treatments (ρ=-0.49, p=0.007) and under placebo (ρ=-0.45, p=0.004). The results showed that the drug effect in the less frequent cannabis users was more pronounced on the SWM (p<0.01) and VWM (p<0.01), whereas there appeared to be little drug effect in the frequent cannabis users. Conclusion: Low doses of synthetic cannabinoid impaired SWM and VWM, indicating that exogenous activation of the cannabinoid system influences cognitive performance. Further, the results replicated previous findings that schizotypy is correlated with deficits in WM. Clinical Trial Registry Name: Nabilone and caffeine effects on the perceptions of visually, auditory, tactile and multimodal illusions in healthy volunteers. Clinical Trial Registration Number: CT-2018-CTN-02561 (Therapeutic Goods Administration Clinical Trial Registry) and ACTRN12618001292268 (The Australian New Zealand Clinical Trials Registry).

7.
Psych J ; 8(1): 90-109, 2019 Mar.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30793518

RESUMO

Drug studies are powerful models to investigate the neuropharmacological mechanisms underlying temporal processing in humans. This study administered dexamphetamine to 24 healthy volunteers to investigate time perception at different time scales, along with contributions from working memory. Healthy volunteers were administered 0.45 mg/kg dexamphetamine or placebo in a double-blind, crossover, placebo-controlled design. Time perception was assessed using three experimental tasks: a time-discrimination task, which asked participants to determine whether a comparison interval (1200 ± 0, 50, 100, 150, 200 ms) was shorter or longer than a standard interval (1200 ms); a retrospective time estimation task, which required participants to verbally estimate time intervals (10, 30, 60, 90 and 120 s) retrospectively; and a prospective time-production task, where participants were required to prospectively monitor the passing of time (10, 30, 60, 90 and 120 s). Working memory was assessed with the backwards digit span. On the discrimination task, there was a change in the proportion of long-to-short responses and reaction times in the dexamphetamine condition (but no association with working memory), consistent with an increase in the speed of an internal pacemaker, and an overestimation of durations in the timing of shorter intervals. There was an interaction between dexamphetamine, working memory, and performance on the estimation and production tasks, whereby increasing digit span scores were associated with decreasing interval estimates and increased produced intervals in the placebo condition, but were associated with increased interval estimates and decreased produced intervals after dexamphetamine administration. These findings indicate that the dexamphetamine-induced increase in the speed of the internal pacemaker was modulated by the basal working memory capacity of each participant. These findings in healthy humans have important implications for the role of dopamine, and its contributions to timing deficits, in models of psychiatric disorders.


Assuntos
Dextroanfetamina/farmacologia , Inibidores da Captação de Dopamina/farmacologia , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Percepção do Tempo/efeitos dos fármacos , Adulto , Estudos Cross-Over , Dextroanfetamina/administração & dosagem , Inibidores da Captação de Dopamina/administração & dosagem , Método Duplo-Cego , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Adulto Jovem
8.
Drug Alcohol Depend ; 194: 447-452, 2019 01 01.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-30502546

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Growing evidence shows cannabis use is associated with lower rates of metabolic dysregulation. Despite cannabis impacting each sex differently, few studies have examined the metabolic profile of male and female cannabis users separately. Our aim was to investigate sex differences in the impact of cannabis use on metabolic syndrome in adults with psychotic illness. METHOD: Data from 1078 men and 735 women interviewed in the second Australian national survey of psychosis were analyzed using multiple logistic regression to model separately, for each sex, the influence of no, occasional and frequent past-year cannabis use on metabolic syndrome, adjusting for potential covariates including antipsychotic medication, smoking, and physical activity. RESULTS: The proportion of women and men with metabolic syndrome was 58.1% and 57.6% respectively. Unadjusted analyses showed frequent cannabis use was associated with significantly lower odds of metabolic syndrome for both sexes. In adjusted analyses, the association between metabolic syndrome and frequent cannabis use remained significant for men (AOR = 0.49, 95% CI = 0.31-0.78), but not for women (AOR = 0.68, 95% CI = 0.37-1.24). Frequent cannabis use was associated with lower odds of abdominal obesity, hypertension and elevated triglyceride levels in men only. CONCLUSIONS: The differences we found suggest cannabinoid regulation of energy balance may be sex-dependent and highlight the importance of examining cannabis use in men and women separately. At the same time, the negative association between cannabis and psychosis onset and relapse should not be dismissed.


Assuntos
Fumar Maconha/fisiopatologia , Síndrome Metabólica/fisiopatologia , Transtornos Psicóticos/fisiopatologia , Caracteres Sexuais , Adolescente , Adulto , Antipsicóticos/uso terapêutico , Austrália , Feminino , Inquéritos Epidemiológicos , Humanos , Masculino , Síndrome Metabólica/complicações , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Transtornos Psicóticos/complicações , Transtornos Psicóticos/tratamento farmacológico , Recidiva , Adulto Jovem
9.
Schizophr Res ; 192: 75-81, 2018 02.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-28416095

RESUMO

Individuals with passivity (first-rank) symptoms report that their actions, thoughts and sensations are influenced or controlled by an external (non-self) agent. Passivity symptoms are closely linked to schizophrenia and related disorders yet they remain poorly understood. One dominant framework posits a role for deficits in the sense of agency. An important question is whether deficits in self-agency can be differentiated from other-agency in schizophrenia and passivity symptoms. This study aimed to evaluate self- and other-agency in 51 people with schizophrenia (n=20 current, 10 past, 21 no history of passivity symptoms), and 48 healthy controls. Participants completed the projected hand illusion (PHI) with active and passive movements, as well as immediate and delayed visual feedback. Experiences of agency and loss of agency over the participant's hand and the image ('the other hand') were assessed with a self-report questionnaire. Those with passivity symptoms (current and past) reported less difference in agency between active and passive movements on items assessing agency over their own hand (but not agency over the other hand). Relative to the healthy controls, the current and never groups continued to experience the illusion with delayed visual feedback suggesting impaired timing mechanisms regardless of symptom profile. These findings are consistent with a reduced contribution of proprioceptive predictive cues to agency judgements specific to self representations in people with passivity symptoms, and a subsequent reliance on external visual cues in these judgements. Altogether, these findings emphasise the multifactorial nature of agency and the contribution of multiple impairments to passivity symptoms.


Assuntos
Imagem Corporal , Mãos , Ilusões , Transtornos Psicóticos/psicologia , Psicologia do Esquizofrênico , Adulto , Feminino , Humanos , Julgamento , Modelos Lineares , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Modelos Psicológicos , Atividade Motora , Movimento , Propriocepção , Transtornos Psicóticos/fisiopatologia , Esquizofrenia/fisiopatologia , Autorrelato , Percepção Visual
10.
PLoS One ; 11(12): e0167220, 2016.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27936175

RESUMO

The effects of early life stress in utero or in neonates has long-term consequences on hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) stress axis function and neurodevelopment. These effects extend into adulthood and may underpin a variety of mental illnesses and be related to various developmental and cognitive changes. We examined the potential role of neonatal HPA axis activation on adult psychopathology and dopamine sensitivity in the mature rat using neonatal exposure to the synthetic glucocorticoid receptor agonist and stress hormone, dexamethasone. We utilized a comprehensive battery of assessments for behaviour, brain function and gene expression to determine if elevated early life HPA activation is associated with adult-onset neuropsychiatric traits. Dexamethasone exposure increased startle reactivity under all conditions tested, but decreased sensitivity of sensorimotor gating to dopaminergic disruption-contrasting with what is observed in several neuropsychiatric diseases. Under certain conditions there also appeared to be mild long-term changes in stress and anxiety-related behaviours with neonatal dexamethasone exposure. Electrophysiology revealed that there were no consistent neuropsychiatric abnormalities in auditory processing or resting state brain function with dexamethasone exposure. However, neonatal dexamethasone altered auditory cortex glucocorticoid activation, and auditory cortex synchronization. Our results indicate that neonatal HPA axis activation by dexamethasone alters several aspects of adult brain function and behaviour and may induce long-term changes in emotional stress-reactivity. However, neonatal dexamethasone exposure is not specifically related to any particular neuropsychiatric disease.


Assuntos
Dexametasona/farmacologia , Comportamento Exploratório/efeitos dos fármacos , Sistema Hipotálamo-Hipofisário/efeitos dos fármacos , Sistema Hipófise-Suprarrenal/efeitos dos fármacos , Reflexo de Sobressalto/efeitos dos fármacos , Fatores Etários , Animais , Animais Recém-Nascidos , Ansiedade/fisiopatologia , Ansiedade/psicologia , Córtex Auditivo/efeitos dos fármacos , Córtex Auditivo/metabolismo , Córtex Auditivo/fisiopatologia , Encéfalo/efeitos dos fármacos , Encéfalo/fisiopatologia , Eletroencefalografia , Potenciais Evocados Auditivos/genética , Expressão Gênica/efeitos dos fármacos , Glucocorticoides/farmacologia , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Ratos Sprague-Dawley , Receptores de Dopamina D1/genética , Receptores de Dopamina D2/genética , Reação em Cadeia da Polimerase Via Transcriptase Reversa , Estresse Psicológico/fisiopatologia
11.
Cogn Neuropsychiatry ; 21(4): 354-368, 2016 07.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27646600

RESUMO

INTRODUCTION: Individuals with schizophrenia, particularly those with passivity symptoms, often feel that their actions and thoughts are controlled by an external agent. Recent evidence has elucidated the role of body representations in the aetiology of passivity symptoms, yet one representation - body structural description - has not yet been examined. Additionally, body image has rarely been examined outside of bodily illusions (e.g., rubber hand experiments) and external validation is required. METHODS: Body structural description was assessed with an in-between task and a matching body parts by location task, and body image with a questionnaire examining body distortion experiences (containing subscales assessing boundary loss, depersonalisation and body size distortions). Individuals with schizophrenia (20 with current, 12 with past and 21 with no history of passivity symptoms) and 48 healthy controls participated in the study. RESULTS: People with schizophrenia (as a group) made more errors on the in-between task, but not on the matching body parts by location task. Individuals with current passivity symptoms reported greater distortions on all subscales relative to the other clinical samples, except for experiences of boundary loss which were common to both passivity symptom groups. CONCLUSIONS: The results indicate that body structural description may be altered in schizophrenia generally and body image alterations are worsened in passivity symptoms, and these alterations likely contribute to the emergence of passivity symptoms.


Assuntos
Imagem Corporal/psicologia , Transtornos da Percepção/psicologia , Distorção da Percepção , Esquizofrenia/complicações , Psicologia do Esquizofrênico , Adulto , Estudos de Casos e Controles , Despersonalização/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
12.
Conscious Cogn ; 45: 9-23, 2016 10.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-27544689

RESUMO

Passivity symptoms in schizophrenia are characterised by an absence of agency for actions, thoughts and other somatic experiences. Time perception and intentional binding have both been linked to agency and schizophrenia but have not been examined in passivity symptoms. Time perception and intentional binding were assessed in people with schizophrenia (n=15 with, n=24 without passivity symptoms) and 43 healthy controls using an interval estimation procedure (200, 400 and 600ms intervals) with active, passive and observed movements. People with passivity symptoms did not display action-modulation of time perception, while those without passivity symptoms estimated intervals to be the same after active and observed movements. Additionally, both clinical samples reported intervals to be shorter with increasing interval length. We propose that impaired predictive processes may produce an overreliance on external cues and, together with shorter perceived intervals, lead to the subjective loss of agency.


Assuntos
Conscientização/fisiologia , Emoções/fisiologia , Julgamento/fisiologia , Esquizofrenia/fisiopatologia , Psicologia do Esquizofrênico , Percepção do Tempo/fisiologia , Adulto , Sinais (Psicologia) , Feminino , Humanos , Intenção , Masculino , Desempenho Psicomotor
13.
Hum Brain Mapp ; 37(2): 570-88, 2016 Feb.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-26577247

RESUMO

The catecholamines-dopamine and noradrenaline-play important roles in directing and guiding behavior. Disorders of these systems, particularly within the dopamine system, are associated with several severe and chronically disabling psychiatric and neurological disorders. We used the recently published group independent components analysis (ICA) procedure outlined by Chen et al. (2013) to present the first pharmaco-EEG ICA analysis of the resting-state EEG in healthy participants administered 0.45 mg/kg dexamphetamine. Twenty-eight healthy participants between 18 and 41 were recruited. Bayesian nested-domain models that explicitly account for spatial and functional relationships were used to contrast placebo and dexamphetamine on component spectral power and several connectivity metrics. Dexamphetamine led to reductions across delta, theta, and alpha spectral power bands that were predominantly localized to Frontal and Central regions. Beta 1 and beta 2 power were reduced by dexamphetamine at Frontal ICs, while beta 2 and gamma power was enhanced by dexamphetamine in posterior regions, including the parietal, occipital-temporal, and occipital regions. Power-power coupling under dexamphetamine was similar for both states, resembling the eyes open condition under placebo. However, orthogonalized measures of power coupling and phase coupling did not show the same effect of dexamphetamine as power-power coupling. We discuss the alterations of low- and high-frequency EEG power in response to dexamphetamine within the context of disorders of dopamine regulation, in particular schizophrenia, as well as in the context of a recently hypothesized association between low-frequency power and aspects of anhedonia. Hum Brain Mapp 37:570-588, 2016. © 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc.


Assuntos
Encéfalo/efeitos dos fármacos , Encéfalo/fisiologia , Dextroanfetamina/farmacologia , Inibidores da Captação de Dopamina/farmacologia , Adulto , Ritmo alfa/efeitos dos fármacos , Teorema de Bayes , Ritmo Delta/efeitos dos fármacos , Eletroencefalografia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Vias Neurais/efeitos dos fármacos , Vias Neurais/fisiologia , Descanso , Processamento de Sinais Assistido por Computador , Ritmo Teta/efeitos dos fármacos , Adulto Jovem
14.
J Neurosci Methods ; 243: 1-7, 2015 Mar 30.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25593048

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: Infrared (IR) technology is a flexible and effective way of measuring animal locomotion. However, the cost of most commercial IR equipment can limit their availability. We have designed an inexpensive and effective replacement for commercial IR sensors that can be attached to enclosures to monitor animal locomotion. NEW METHOD: IR components were soldered to circuits connected to a single microcontroller. These IR components were housed inexpensively using plastic tubing and cork discs to further focus and extend detection of the IR beam. A standard personal computer recorded data from circuit boards connected to an inexpensive interface. This system may be used in a range of lighting conditions without requiring readjustment or recalibration. RESULTS: Validation of our equipment design was done with male Sprague Dawley rats treated with reserpine 22h prior to administration of saline or l-DOPA (125mg/kg). Data was collected in eight different measures: horizontal activity, immobile time, elevated activity, centre elevated activity, elevation time, elevation bout, and repeated and non-repeated movement while elevated. l-DOPA increased horizontal movement and all elevated activity excepting elevated movement and centre elevated movement, demonstrating selective drug effects. COMPARISON WITH EXISTING METHODS: The total cost of our complete IR system (US$517.45) was substantially less than the least expensive quote (US$19,666.90) obtained for a commercial IR system. CONCLUSIONS: We have successfully designed and constructed a flexible and inexpensive IR system to monitor at least eight measures of rodent locomotion at a significantly lesser cost than quoted by commercial suppliers.


Assuntos
Actigrafia/instrumentação , Comportamento Animal , Raios Infravermelhos , Locomoção , Actigrafia/economia , Inibidores da Captação Adrenérgica/farmacologia , Animais , Comportamento Animal/efeitos dos fármacos , Computadores/economia , Dopaminérgicos/farmacologia , Desenho de Equipamento , Levodopa/farmacologia , Iluminação , Locomoção/efeitos dos fármacos , Masculino , Microtecnologia/instrumentação , Atividade Motora/efeitos dos fármacos , Distribuição Aleatória , Ratos Sprague-Dawley , Reserpina/farmacologia , Software
15.
Atten Percept Psychophys ; 77(1): 207-19, 2015 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25120179

RESUMO

The projected hand illusion (PHI) is a variant of the rubber hand illusion (RHI), and both are commonly used to study mechanisms of self-perception. A questionnaire was developed by Longo et al. (2008) to measure qualitative changes in the RHI. Such psychometric analyses have not yet been conducted on the questionnaire for the PHI. The present study is an attempt to validate minor modifications of the questionnaire of Longo et al. to assess the PHI in a community sample (n = 48) and to determine the association with selected demographic (age, sex, years of education), cognitive (Digit Span), and clinical (psychotic-like experiences) variables. Principal components analysis on the questionnaire data extracted four components: Embodiment of "Other" Hand, Disembodiment of Own Hand, Deafference, and Agency-in both synchronous and asynchronous PHI conditions. Questions assessing "Embodiment" and "Agency" loaded onto orthogonal components. Greater illusion ratings were positively associated with being female, being younger, and having higher scores on psychotic-like experiences. There was no association with cognitive performance. Overall, this study confirmed that self-perception as measured with PHI is a multicomponent construct, similar in many respects to the RHI. The main difference lies in the separation of Embodiment and Agency into separate constructs, and this likely reflects the fact that the "live" image of the PHI presents a more realistic picture of the hand and of the stroking movements of the experimenter compared with the RHI.


Assuntos
Mãos , Ilusões/psicologia , Autoimagem , Adulto , Atenção/fisiologia , Cognição/fisiologia , Delusões/psicologia , Feminino , Humanos , Ilusões/fisiologia , Masculino , Memória de Curto Prazo/fisiologia , Pessoa de Meia-Idade , Movimento/fisiologia , Psicometria , Fatores Sexuais , Inquéritos e Questionários , Adulto Jovem
16.
Front Psychiatry ; 5: 126, 2014.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25309460

RESUMO

Individuals with schizophrenia, particularly those with passivity symptoms, may not feel in control of their actions, believing them to be controlled by external agents. Cognitive operations that contribute to these symptoms may include abnormal processing in agency as well as body representations that deal with body schema and body image. However, these operations in schizophrenia are not fully understood, and the questions of general versus specific deficits in individuals with different symptom profiles remain unanswered. Using the projected-hand illusion (a digital video version of the rubber-hand illusion) with synchronous and asynchronous stroking (500 ms delay), and a hand laterality judgment task, we assessed sense of agency, body image, and body schema in 53 people with clinically stable schizophrenia (with a current, past, and no history of passivity symptoms) and 48 healthy controls. The results revealed a stable trait in schizophrenia with no difference between clinical subgroups (sense of agency) and some quantitative (specific) differences depending on the passivity symptom profile (body image and body schema). Specifically, a reduced sense of self-agency was a common feature of all clinical subgroups. However, subgroup comparisons showed that individuals with passivity symptoms (both current and past) had significantly greater deficits on tasks assessing body image and body schema, relative to the other groups. In addition, patients with current passivity symptoms failed to demonstrate the normal reduction in body illusion typically seen with a 500 ms delay in visual feedback (asynchronous condition), suggesting internal timing problems. Altogether, the results underscore self-abnormalities in schizophrenia, provide evidence for both trait abnormalities and state changes specific to passivity symptoms, and point to a role for internal timing deficits as a mechanistic explanation for external cues becoming a possible source of self-body input.

17.
Behav Brain Res ; 275: 225-33, 2014 Dec 15.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-25200515

RESUMO

Many factors influence neurodevelopment. However, their contribution to adult neural function is often unclear. This is often due to complex expression profiles, cell signalling, neuroanatomy, and a lack of effective tests to assess the function of neural circuits in vivo. Ephrin-A2 and ephrin-A5 are cell surface proteins implicated in multiple aspects of neurodevelopment. While the role of ephrin-As in visual, auditory and learning behaviours has been explored, little is known about their role in dopaminergic and neuromotor pathways, despite expression in associated brain regions. Here we probe the function of ephrin-A2 and ephrin-A5 in the development of the dopaminergic and neuromotor pathways using counts of tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) positive cells in the substantia nigra pars compacta (SNpc) and the ventral tegmental area (VTA), the acoustic startle reflex (ASR), and a measure of sensorimotor gating, prepulse inhibition (PPI). Analysis of the ASR and PPI in ephrin-A2 and/or ephrin-A5 knock-out mice revealed that both genes play distinct roles in mediating ASR circuits, but are unlikely to play a role in PPI. Knock-out of either gene resulted in robust changes in startle response magnitude and measures of startle onset and peak latencies. However, ephrin-A2 and ephrin-A5 regulate aspects of the ASR differently: ephrin-A2 KO mice have increased startle amplitude, increased sensitivity and reduced latency to startle, whilst ephrin-A5 KO mice show opposite effects. Neither of the gene knock outs affected PPI, despite ephrin-A5 KO mice showing changes in dopamine cell numbers in nuclei thought to regulate PPI. We propose that majority of the changes observed ephrin-A2 and ephrin-A5 KO mice appear to be mediated by the effects on motor neurons and their muscle targets, rather than changes in auditory sensitivity.


Assuntos
Efrina-A2/deficiência , Efrina-A5/deficiência , Inibição Neural/genética , Filtro Sensorial/genética , Estimulação Acústica , Análise de Variância , Animais , Efrina-A2/genética , Efrina-A5/genética , Masculino , Camundongos , Camundongos Endogâmicos C57BL , Camundongos Knockout , Parte Compacta da Substância Negra/metabolismo , Tempo de Reação/genética , Reflexo de Sobressalto/genética , Tirosina 3-Mono-Oxigenase/metabolismo , Área Tegmentar Ventral/metabolismo
18.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) ; 231(22): 4349-59, 2014 Nov.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24800893

RESUMO

RATIONALE: Recent case reports describe recreational use of quetiapine and drug-seeking behaviour to obtain quetiapine, an atypical antipsychotic. OBJECTIVE: We examined the hypothesis that quetiapine (10, 20 or 40 mg/kg) alone or co-administered with (+)-amphetamine (0.25, 0.5, 0.75 or 2.0 mg/kg) will affect reward and/or decrease anxiety in rats, as measured by conditioned place preference (CPP) and elevated plus maze (EPM) test, respectively. RESULTS: Quetiapine (20 mg/kg) produced greater open arm time and entries in the EPM test compared to 10 and 40 mg/kg, and quetiapine (10 mg/kg) significantly increased open arm entries and time when co-administered with (+)-amphetamine (0.5 mg/kg) compared to (+)-amphetamine (0.5 mg/kg) alone, suggesting decreased anxiety. Quetiapine (10, 20 or 40 mg/kg) produced no CPP when administered alone; the lowest dose of quetiapine (10 mg/kg) reduced CPP produced by a low dose of (+)-amphetamine (0.25 mg/kg), but had no significant effect on CPP produced by a higher dose (0.5 mg/kg). DISCUSSION: The quetiapine-induced anxiolytic effect in the EPM might explain why humans are misusing quetiapine and combining it with (+)-amphetamine. It is possible that humans experience an anxiolytic effect of the combined drugs and relatively unaltered rewarding effects of (+)-amphetamine. The results shed some light on the question of why humans are abusing and misusing quetiapine, despite its dopamine (DA) D2 receptor antagonism; it will be the task of future studies to identify the pharmacological mechanism mediating this behaviour.


Assuntos
Anfetamina/farmacologia , Antipsicóticos/farmacologia , Ansiedade , Comportamento Animal/efeitos dos fármacos , Estimulantes do Sistema Nervoso Central/farmacologia , Dibenzotiazepinas/farmacologia , Recompensa , Anfetamina/administração & dosagem , Animais , Antipsicóticos/administração & dosagem , Estimulantes do Sistema Nervoso Central/administração & dosagem , Condicionamento Psicológico/efeitos dos fármacos , Dibenzotiazepinas/administração & dosagem , Sinergismo Farmacológico , Masculino , Aprendizagem em Labirinto/efeitos dos fármacos , Fumarato de Quetiapina , Ratos , Ratos Wistar
19.
Psychopharmacology (Berl) ; 231(11): 2327-37, 2014 Jun.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-24346114

RESUMO

RATIONALE: Amphetamine challenge in rodent prepulse inhibition (PPI) studies has been used to model potential dopamine involvement in effects that may be relevant to schizophrenia, though similar studies in healthy humans have failed to report replicable or robust effects. OBJECTIVES: The present study investigated dexamphetamine effects on PPI in healthy humans with an increased dose and a range of startling stimulus intensities to determine participants' sensitivity and range of responses to the stimuli. METHODS: A randomised, placebo-controlled dexamphetamine (0.45 mg/kg, per os.), double-blind, counterbalanced, within-subject design was used. PPI was measured in 64 participants across a range of startling stimulus intensities, during two attention set conditions (ATTEND and IGNORE). Startle magnitudes for pulse-alone and prepulse-pulse magnitudes were modelled using the startle reflex magnitude (sigmoid) function. Parameters were extracted from these fits, including the upper limit of the asymptote (maximum startle reflex capacity, R MAX), intensity threshold, stimulus intensity that elicits a half-maximal response (ES50) and the maximum rate of change of startle response magnitude to an increase in stimulus intensity. RESULTS: Dexamphetamine increased the threshold and ES50 of the response to pulse-alone trials in both sexes and reduced R MAX exclusively in females. Dexamphetamine modestly increased PPI of the R MAX across both attention conditions. PPI of R MAX was reduced during the ATTEND condition compared to the IGNORE condition. CONCLUSIONS: Results indicate that sex differences exist in motor, but not sensory, components of the startle reflex. Findings also reveal that administration of 0.45 mg/kg dexamphetamine to healthy humans does not mimic PPI effects observed in schizophrenia.


Assuntos
Estimulantes do Sistema Nervoso Central/farmacologia , Dextroanfetamina/farmacologia , Inibição Pré-Pulso/efeitos dos fármacos , Reflexo de Sobressalto/efeitos dos fármacos , Caracteres Sexuais , Estimulação Acústica , Atenção/efeitos dos fármacos , Atenção/fisiologia , Método Duplo-Cego , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Testes Neuropsicológicos , Inibição Pré-Pulso/fisiologia , Desempenho Psicomotor/efeitos dos fármacos , Desempenho Psicomotor/fisiologia , Psicofarmacologia , Reflexo de Sobressalto/fisiologia , Adulto Jovem
20.
J Psychiatry Neurosci ; 38(1): 24-32, 2013 Jan.
Artigo em Inglês | MEDLINE | ID: mdl-22894820

RESUMO

BACKGROUND: An emerging endophenotype of schizophrenia is the reduction of both power and phase locking of the 40 Hz auditory steady state response (ASSR), and there have been a number of reports linking increased γ activity with positive psychotic symptoms. Schizophrenia and, more specifically, positive psychotic symptoms have been closely linked to increased dopamine (DA) neurophysiology. Therefore, we gave dexamphetamine to healthy participants to determine the effect that increased DA transmission would have on the ASSR. METHODS: We administered 0.45 mg/kg of dexamphetamine orally in a double-blind placebo-controlled crossover study. Stimuli were 20 Hz and 40 Hz click trains presented in an auditory oddball-type stimulus format (probability of stimulus presentation: 0.2 for targets, 0.8 for nontargets). RESULTS: We included 44 healthy volunteers (18 women) in the study. Dexamphetamine significantly increased the 40 Hz power for both target and nontarget ASSR stimuli. Dexamphetamine did not significantly affect the 40 Hz phase-locking factor (PLF) or the 20 Hz power and PLF. Whereas there were significant effects of selective attention on power and PLF for 20 and 40 Hz ASSR, there were no significant interactions between dexamphetamine and selective attention. LIMITATIONS: Dexamphetamine releases both noradrenaline and DA with equal potency. Further research with selective dopaminergic and noradrenergic agents will better characterize the effects of monoamines on γ activity. CONCLUSION: The results demonstrate a frequency-specific effect of dexamphetamine on the ASSR. This finding is consistent with previous research that has found an association between increased γ and positive symptoms of psychosis. However, this result also raises the possibility that previous 40 Hz ASSR findings in people with schizophrenia may be confounded by effects of antipsychotic medication. Possible neural mechanisms by which dexamphetamine specifically increases 40 Hz power are also discussed. AUSTRALIAN AND NEW ZEALAND CLINICAL TRIALS REGISTRY NUMBER: ACTRN12608000610336.


Assuntos
Estimulantes do Sistema Nervoso Central/farmacologia , Dextroanfetamina/farmacologia , Potenciais Evocados Auditivos/efeitos dos fármacos , Estimulação Acústica/métodos , Adulto , Córtex Auditivo/efeitos dos fármacos , Estudos Cross-Over , Método Duplo-Cego , Eletroencefalografia , Feminino , Humanos , Masculino , Pessoa de Meia-Idade
SELEÇÃO DE REFERÊNCIAS
DETALHE DA PESQUISA
...